Are Cravings Trying to Tell Us Something? The Science Behind the Urge
- Caterina Pascale
- 2 hours ago
- 7 min read

Are Cravings Trying to Tell Us Something? The Science Behind the Urge
Unpacking the truth about food cravings—do they reveal hidden nutrient needs or just temptations of the mind?
by Catherina Pascale
Abstract
Food cravings are an almost universal human experience, yet their biological significance remains poorly understood. Although it is commonly thought that cravings reflect unmet nutritional needs—such as craving red meat for iron or chocolate for magnesium—scientific research show that there are multiple aspects to consider. This article explores the psychological, physiological, cultural, and neurobiological factors underlying cravings and examines the extent to which they align with the body’s actual nutritional requirements. Drawing from studies in nutrition, psychology, and neuroscience, we conclude that while cravings may sometimes correlate with deficiencies, they are more often shaped by learned associations, emotional states, and pleasure-seeking desires.
Introduction
Cravings are intense desires for specific foods and are typically distinguished from general hunger by their specificity and urgency. Unlike hunger, which is a biological signal for caloric need, cravings are often associated with particular tastes, textures, or food categories—commonly high-calorie, sugar-laden, or salty foods (Weingarten & Elston, 1991). Popular culture and some health advocates have perpetuated the idea that cravings are our body's way of signalling nutrient deficiencies. But is this idea scientifically valid? Do cravings guide us toward dietary balance, or are they more psychological than physiological?
Defining Cravings
Cravings can be categorized into two major types:
Homeostatic cravings: These may arise when the body lacks specific nutrients or energy, potentially reflecting a biological need.
Hedonic cravings: These are driven by the pleasure of eating, irrespective of physiological need.
It is essential to distinguish between the two, as the mechanisms and implications differ significantly (Berthoud, 2004).
The Psychology of Cravings
The majority of studies point toward the psychological underpinnings of cravings. Emotional states—such as stress, boredom, and anxiety—frequently trigger cravings, particularly for “comfort foods” (Macht, 2008). Emotional eating is well-documented and can occur independently of caloric or nutrient needs.
Food cravings are more strongly linked to mood factors such as boredom and anxiety than to dietary restraint or food deprivation. The study by Hill et al. (1991) found that emotional and external eating, rather than dieting, are key contributors to craving episodes.
Cultural and Learned Influences
Cravings are strongly shaped by culture and food availability. Dietary behaviors are shaped by a complex interplay of personal preferences, cultural influences, social dynamics, and broader environmental, economic, and policy factors. This indicates that cravings are not universally hardwired but are learned and reinforced over time (Jaysinghe, et. al., 2025).
Conditioned responses play a role, too. If someone routinely eats popcorn during movies, they may begin to crave popcorn at the sight of a cinema, regardless of hunger or nutritional need. These conditioned cues trigger the brain’s reward circuitry, particularly the dopamine system (Volkow et al., 2013).
Cravings and Nutrient Deficiencies: Is There a Link?
Despite the psychological and cultural influences, can some cravings truly signal nutrient deficiencies? Anecdotal and clinical evidence suggest a few cases where this may be true:
1. Pica and Micronutrient Deficiency
The most clear-cut example of a craving reflecting nutritional need is pica—a condition characterized by the compulsive ingestion of non-food items such as dirt, clay, or ice. Pica is most often associated with iron deficiency anemia and, to a lesser extent, zinc deficiency (Young, 2010).
In pregnant women, ice cravings (pagophagia) have been strongly linked to iron-deficiency anemia, and supplementation often resolves the craving (Hunt, et. al., 2009). However, the mechanisms are not entirely understood—ice chewing may improve alertness in anemic individuals by increasing blood flow to the brain, rather than directly addressing iron deficiency.
2. Salt Craving and Addison’s Disease
In individuals with Addison’s disease (adrenal insufficiency), salt cravings may arise due to low aldosterone levels, which impair sodium retention (Betterle et al., 2002). These cases are rare but support the notion that some cravings may serve a physiological purpose.
3. Cravings During Pregnancy
Pregnancy is frequently accompanied by cravings, and some researchers have suggested a possible nutritional rationale. Cravings for dairy or red meat could theoretically reflect increased needs for calcium or iron. However, evidence is inconsistent. A study by Orloff and Hormes (2014) found no consistent relationship between food cravings in pregnancy and nutritional status.
The Neurobiology of Craving
The brain’s reward system, particularly the mesolimbic dopamine pathway, plays a central role in cravings. Food cues (e.g., sight, smell, or even thought of food) activate the nucleus accumbens, releasing dopamine and producing a sensation of anticipation or pleasure (Berridge & Robinson, 2003).
Importantly, this response is often stronger for high-sugar or high-fat foods, even in the absence of hunger. Functional MRI studies show that individuals with obesity have heightened reward responses to food images, suggesting that pleasure-seeking signaling can overpower homeostatic needs (Stoeckel et al., 2008).
Hormones such as ghrelin (hunger hormone) and leptin (satiety hormone) modulate these neural pathways. When leptin levels are low (e.g., during caloric restriction), cravings and food-seeking behaviors increase (Morton et al., 2006). However, it is interesting to note that these changes are not necessarily directed at the most nutritious foods but often toward the most palatable ones.
Sugar Cravings: A Special Case?
One of the most commonly craved categories is sugar. There is ongoing debate about whether sugar is addictive in the same way as drugs. Animal studies have shown that intermittent access to sugar can produce bingeing behavior and withdrawal-like symptoms (Avena et al., 2008). However, translating these findings to humans is controversial.
Human studies show that sugar activates reward pathways in a manner similar to addictive substances but lacks the pharmacological intensity of drugs such as cocaine or heroin. The "addiction" to sugar may be more behavioral and psychological than neurochemical (Westwater et al., 2016).
Hormonal and Gender Differences
Women report cravings more frequently than men, and hormonal fluctuations may be a contributing factor. The luteal phase of the menstrual cycle, characterized by increased progesterone and decreased serotonin, is often associated with heightened cravings—especially for carbohydrates (Dye & Blundell, 1997).
This supports the theory that some cravings may be attempts to self-regulate neurotransmitter levels, such as boosting serotonin through carbohydrate intake. However, this remains a hypothesis rather than a proven mechanism.
Cravings in Eating Disorders
Cravings also feature prominently in eating disorders such as binge-eating disorder (BED) and bulimia nervosa. Individuals with these disorders often experience intense cravings and a loss of control over eating.
Crucially, studies show that these cravings are rarely aligned with nutritional deficiencies but rather with altered reward sensitivity (Polivy, et. al., 2005).
Cognitive-behavioral therapies aimed at disrupting the craving-response cycle have shown efficacy in reducing binge episodes, further suggesting a psychological rather than physiological origin (Wilson et al., 2007).
Interventions to Manage Cravings
Given that most cravings are not reflective of true nutritional needs, strategies for managing them tend to focus on behavioral and cognitive interventions:
Mindfulness training has shown promise in reducing the intensity and frequency of cravings by promoting awareness without judgment (Alberts et al., 2010).
Cognitive distraction (e.g., engaging in a non-food-related activity) can reduce cravings in the given moment.
Exposure therapy, where individuals are exposed to craved foods without consuming them, can desensitize cue-reactivity over time.
All considered, it is important to seek medical attention in cases where deficiencies are suspected (e.g., anemia, pregnancy), appropriate supplementation should be medically guided.
Conclusion
Cravings are complex phenomena which are influenced by a number of factors and origins. While certain extreme cravings—such as those seen in pica or Addison’s disease—may genuinely reflect physiological imbalances, the vast majority of cravings do not indicate a specific nutrient deficiency. Instead, they are more often driven by psychological, emotional, cultural, and neurobiological factors.
The idea that our bodies "know" what they need and express that through cravings is an appealing notion, but one that lacks strong empirical support for most common cravings. Understanding the true nature of cravings can help us respond to them more wisely—by distinguishing between a biological need and a psychological urge.
References
Alberts, H. J., Mulkens, S., Smeets, M., & Thewissen, R. (2010). Coping with food cravings. Investigating the potential of a mindfulness-based intervention. Appetite, 55(1), 160–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.05.044 .
Avena, N. M., Rada, P., & Hoebel, B. G. (2008). Evidence for sugar addiction: behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews,32(1), 20–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.019
Berthoud H. R. (2004). Mind versus metabolism in the control of food intake and energy balance. Physiology & behavior, 81(5), 781–793.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.034
Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (2003). Parsing reward. Trends in neurosciences, 26(9), 507–513. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9
Betterle, C., Dal Pra, C., Mantero, F., & Zanchetta, R. (2002). Autoimmune adrenal insufficiency and autoimmune polyendocrine syndromes: autoantibodies, autoantigens, and their applicability in diagnosis and disease prediction. Endocrine reviews, 23(3), 327–364. https://doi.org/10.1210/edrv.23.3.0466
Dye, L., & Blundell, J. E. (1997). Menstrual cycle and appetite control: implications for weight regulation. Human reproduction (Oxford, England), 12(6), 1142–1151. https://doi.org/10.1093/humrep/12.6.1142
Hill, A. J., Weaver, C. F., & Blundell, J. E. (1991). Food craving, dietary restraint and mood. Appetite, 17(3), 187–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/0195-6663(91)90021-j
Hunt, J. R., Zito, C. A., & Johnson, L. K. (2009). Body iron excretion by healthy men and women. The American journal of clinical nutrition, 89(6), 1792–1798. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.2009.27439
Jayasinghe, S., Byrne, N. M., & Hills, A. P. (2025). Cultural influences on dietary choices. Progress in cardiovascular diseases, S0033-0620(25)00020-9. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pcad.2025.02.003
Macht M. (2008). How emotions affect eating: a five-way model. Appetite, 50(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002
Morton, G. J., Cummings, D. E., Baskin, D. G., Barsh, G. S., & Schwartz, M. W. (2006). Central nervous system control of food intake and body weight. Nature, 443(7109), 289–295. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05026
Orloff, N. C., & Hormes, J. M. (2014). Pickles and ice cream! Food cravings in pregnancy: hypotheses, preliminary evidence, and directions for future research. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 1076. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01076
Polivy, J., Coleman, J., & Herman, C. P. (2005). The effect of deprivation on food cravings and eating behavior in restrained and unrestrained eaters. The International journal of eating disorders, 38(4), 301–309. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20195
Stoeckel, L. E., Weller, R. E., Cook, E. W., 3rd, Twieg, D. B., Knowlton, R. C., & Cox, J. E. (2008). Widespread reward-system activation in obese women in response to pictures of high-calorie foods. NeuroImage, 41(2), 636–647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.031
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Tomasi, D., & Baler, R. D. (2013). The addictive dimensionality of obesity. Biological psychiatry, 73(9), 811–818. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.020
Weingarten, H. P., & Elston, D. (1991). Food cravings in a college population. Appetite, 17(3), 167–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/0195-6663(91)90019-o
Westwater, M. L., Fletcher, P. C., & Ziauddeen, H. (2016). Sugar addiction: the state of the science. European journal of nutrition, 55(Suppl 2), 55–69. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-016-1229-6
Wilson, G. T., Grilo, C. M., & Vitousek, K. M. (2007). Psychological treatment of eating disorders. The American psychologist, 62(3), 199–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.3.199
Young S. L. (2010). Pica in pregnancy: new ideas about an old condition. Annual review of nutrition, 30, 403–422. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.nutr.012809.104713
Assessed and Endorsed by the MedReport Medical Review Board